It seems part of our natural inclination toward idolatry and hubris that we repeatedly compare our minds to some technology we have created. There have been many examples of this throughout history, involving new, complex or marvelous inventions as the subject of the comparison. Thus over the centuries the workings of the human mind have been compared to that of steam engines, light bulbs, electrical currents, telephone switchboards, and more recently, computers.
Current
attempts by some neuroscientists to reduce mental and even spiritual phenomena
to an elaborate calculation involving genetics and neurochemistry at the
cellular level is also an example of this metaphorical thinking. But it is an
idolatry; the transcendent quality of the human mind and soul will hardly be
lodged in the interstices of matter. In
our self-congratulatory awe of the machines we have created we forget that we
ourselves our created. “What a piece of
work is man!” as Shakespeare has Hamlet say.
To be fair, the human mind can seem overwhelmingly complex both in structure
and activity. In situations like this, it
is common to use metaphorical thinking in order to understand an entity that is
subtle and mysterious.
Metaphorical
thinking attempts to explain the unknown in terms of the known. It chiefly involves drawing superficial comparisons
between something not fully understood and something that is concrete and
familiar, or at least more readily comprehended. Metaphorical thinking does not produce
knowledge per se, though it often masquerades as such. As in poetry, it serves principally to alter
one’s perspective or point of view—at least one’s appreciation—of some
object. Metaphorical thinking is crucial
to politics, advertising, religion, philosophy and psychology.
An
early example of understanding the human mind in terms of a new and popular
technology is described in Terry Castle’s “Phantasmagoria and the Metaphorics
of Modern Reverie”. The extract that I
have is from Ken Gelder’s fascinating anthology of horror criticism, The Horror Reader (2000). Phantasmagoria
is defined these days as “a constantly shifting complex succession of things
seen or imagined”, as Merriam Webster’s
dictionary puts it.
We
have all experienced phantasmagoria in literature, film, at rock concerts, and
after imbibing certain substances.
Castle reminds us that phantasmagoria once specifically referred to a technological
marvel of the early 1800s, a precursor to the modern slide projector, (and
later cinematic technology), called the “magic lantern”. The device was used to stage “ghost shows”
which were immensely popular in France and England at the time. Castle describes the magic lantern, invented
by Athanasius Kircher in the 1600s, as follows:
“Kircher’s
device, from which all of our modern instruments for slide and cinematic projection
derive, consisted of a lantern containing a candle and concave mirror. A tube with a convex lens at each end was
fitted into an opening in the side of the lantern, while a groove in the middle
of the tube held a small image painted on glass. When candlelight was reflected by the concave
mirror onto the first lens, the lens concentrated the light on the image on the
glass slide. The second lens in turn
magnified the illuminated image and projected it onto a wall or gauze
screen. In darkness, with the screen
itself invisible, images could be made to appear like fantastic luminous shapes,
floating inexplicably in the air.”
Castle
goes on to describe the showmanship involved in conducting a typical magic
lantern show, which used scary and gothic imagery from history, mythology and
biblical themes, set to creepy music and sound effects, and projected in sepulchral
locations. The illusions were of
sufficient quality to frighten many in the audience. These shows were very popular, so much so,
that by the 1860s technological advancements created a market for “do-it-yourself”
magic lantern kits for domestic use.
Because
of the technology’s popularity, references to phantasmagoria began to show up
in the horror literature of the time; Castle cites examples from Edgar Allan
Poe’s The Fall of the House of Usher
(1839) and especially Ligeia (1838). Describing the images depicted on tapestries
and drapes in the bridal chamber, the narrator of Ligeia, who is admittedly an imbiber of opium, remarks:
“To
one entering the room, they bore the appearance of simple monstrosities; but
upon a further advance, this appearance gradually departed; and, step by step,
rounded by an endless succession of the ghastly forms which belong to the
superstition of the Norman, or arise in the guilty slumbers of the monk. The phantasmagoric
effect was vastly heightened by the artificial introduction of a strong
continual current of wind behind the draperies—giving a hideous and uneasy
animation to the whole.”
Fans
of H.P. Lovecraft and Clark Ashton Smith can probably recall similar passages
from the more hallucinatory passages of stories like The Dream Quest of Unknown Kadath (1943) or Smith’s Ubbo-Sathla (1933) among others. Though produced nearly a century later, these
stories seem to contain an echo of phantasmagoric imagery.
Clark
Ashton Smith, for example, makes explicit reference to the magic lantern in his
The Red World of Polaris, (an
interesting story retrieved from obscurity in 2003). An alien overlord shares the contents of its
mind with the crew of the starship Alcyone, using a similar device:
“One
of the delegation left the room forthwith, and returned with a singular
instrument, scarcely comparable in its form to anything used on earth, with
many lenses of a transparent material arranged behind each other in a frame of spiral
rods and arabesque filaments…There, as the men gazed, a picture suddenly sprang
into life, as if from the slide of a magic lantern, and filled the entire
opposite face of the wall.”
Another
example of phantasmagoric imagery can be found in the climax of Lovecraft’s The Haunter of the Dark (1936), when the
now supernaturally disordered mind of Robert Blake can no longer produce a
grammatical sentence:
“…There is a monstrous odour…senses
transfigured…boarding at that tower window cracking and giving way…I see it—coming
here—hell-wind—titan blur—black wings—Yog-Sothoth save me—the three-lobed
burning eye…”
Castle
notes that phantasmagoric imagery in literature often indicated the operations
of a diseased, traumatized or intoxicated mind.
From here the technological metaphor appears in psychological
speculations about the nature of hallucinations and perceptions of supernatural
phenomena. Phantasmagoria shows appeared
at a time when the rationalism of the so-called Enlightenment was being applied
to the debunking of ghosts and other supernatural phenomena, as well as the
broader effort to understand the human psyche.
It was a ready-made technological metaphor; soon, supernatural phenomena
were seen as projections of a diseased mind, just as the magic lantern
projected contrived images of ghosts.
That images
of supernatural horror may be projections
of the mind instead of perceptions of external phenomena is seen in many ghost
stories as well as horror, science fiction and fantasy generally. One example that comes to mind is the
Freudian entity that attacks members of the starship crew in the magnificent
1956 film Forbidden Planet. It turns out that a scientist has been using alien
technology to amplify his intelligence, which has also resulted in projecting
and giving form to repressed feelings of anger and rage—“Monsters from the Id!”
Castle
makes the astute point that rationalist attempts to debunk and internalize
supernatural phenomena as products of a disturbed psyche have in effect “spectralized”
the mind. Insofar as frightening
thoughts and images can recur and terrify, the mind may be said to be haunted
and disturbed by phenomena no less real because internal. Instead of explaining away supernatural phenomena,
Castle feels that the rationalists merely blurred the lines between reality and
the products of a disordered mind.
A
special case is the nature of dreams, which Castle does not address in her
article. Nor are there persuasive explanations from the rationalist camp for
these nightly phenomena that we all experience.
In our dreams we create entire worlds ex nihilo with which we interact
as if they were real, in which “we live and move and have our being.” Where do these worlds come from? What sustains them while we are there? What sustains the real world we think we know?
The
phantasmagoria shows of the early 19th century illustrated the
wonderful tension and ambivalence between supernaturalism and rationalism. As in our movie theatres today, audiences
knew that the illusions were artificially created, technologically
contrived. Yet they were still terrified
by them and sought them out, even applauding improvements in the effectiveness
of the “special effects”. From a
psychological and even religious perspective one can ask why—if we in fact no
longer believe in ghosts, demons, monsters, and the like—we seek ever more graphic
and technical proof of their existence in our horror entertainments. Is it that we are merely trying to get a
closer look at the contents of our minds?
No comments:
Post a Comment
Thank you for your interest in The R'lyeh Tribune! Comments and suggestions are always welcome.